domingo, 15 de mayo de 2016

BioEdge: Does fear change behaviour?

BioEdge: Does fear change behaviour?



Does fear change behaviour?
     
In 2010 the New York City Department of Health launched a grisly 30-second TV ad (above) aimed at reducing the incidence of HIV amongst men who have sex with men. It showed angst-ridden young men reacting to the words osteoporosis, dementia, anal cancer.

The campaign was extremely controversial in the gay community. Some hated it: “it’s sadistic and bullying,” said UK-based journalist Gus Cairns. But gay rights activist Larry Kramer loved it: “these nyc department of health public service announcements are in fact not strong enough!”

These comments represent the two extremes of debate over fear-based public health campaigns. Bioethicists, whose work is supported by the pillar of autonomy, criticise them as stigmatising; public health ethicists, who often take a more utilitarian approach, believe that fear saves lives by motivating people to change their behaviour. From the 1950s to the 1970s, the anti-stigma crowd had had the upper hand. The consensus was that fear did not work.

In the 1980s the question became urgent with the HIV/AIDS epidemic. At first there was bitter resistance to using fear-based advertising to encourage young gays to use condoms. However, deliberately stigmatising warnings have been responsible for declines in the incidence of smoking since the mid-1950s.

An on-line first article in the Journal of Medical Ethics this week asked whether it is time to change tack and recognise that fear does “work”. Campaigns to decrease consumption of cigarettes and soft drinks by using cringe-inducing images appear to have had an impact. Why not HIV? 

“[a meta-analysis in 2015] concluded that fear appeals were effective at positively influencing attitudes, intentions and behaviour; there were very few circumstances under which they were not effective and there were no identifiable circumstances under which they backfired and led to undesirable outcomes…

[T]he long and sometimes bitter debate about the efficacy of fear-based campaigns may be drawing to a close. In thinking about fear-based campaigns in the future, it will be increasingly necessary to ask: not can we, but should we?...

To acknowledge that fear-based campaigns have a role to play is not the equivalent of endorsing efforts that seek to shatter a sense of self. To tug at the gut is not the equivalent of endorsing efforts that seek ‘a total destruction of the individual's status in organized society’.
What about the danger of stigma? The authors respond that fear-based campaigns do not necessarily stigmatise people. In fact they can increase their autonomy as long as they are not deceptive.

Unlike the now dominant perspective within bioethics, a central ethical claim for fear-based efforts was that despite virulent arguments to the contrary, they may in fact enhance autonomous choice… But the justification of appeals to fear and emotion should not be confused with the reliance on outright deception and exaggeration of the risks posed to public health.
- See more at: http://www.bioedge.org/bioethics/does-fear-change-behaviour/11873#sthash.o0th69md.dpuf



Bioedge

The Global Priorities Project and the Future of Humanity Institute, both based at Oxford University, recently produced a Global Catastrophic Risk 2016 report. It’s less gripping than the Left Behind novels about the Second Coming of Christ (with titles like The Rapture: In the Twinkling of an Eye/Countdown to the Earth's Last Days), but, in its own dry, detached way, no less scary.
According to the Oxford experts’ calculations, extinction of the whole human race is reasonably likely.  Scientists have suggested that the risk is 0.1% per year, and perhaps as much as 0.2%. While this may not seem worthwhile worrying about, these figures actually imply, says the report, that “an individual would be more than five times as likely to die in an extinction event than a car crash”.
What sort of calamities are we talking about? Collision with an asteroid, the eruption of a super-volcano, extreme climate change, a bio-engineered pandemic, or even a super-intelligent computer declaring war on wetware humanity.
Tiny probabilities add up, so that the chance of extinction in the next century is 9.5% -- which is worth worrying about. And of course, a mere global catastrophe, involving the death of a tenth of the population, is far more likely. That is a very startling statistic.
However, even at Oxford they make mistakes. Within days of issuing the Global Catastrophic Risk 2016 report, the experts were eating humble pie. A mathematician reviewed its calculations and concluded that “the Future of Humanity Institute seems very confused re: the future of humanity”. The authors had to give more nuance and context to their most startling statistic. It doesn’t inspire a lot of confidence in the ethics of existential risk. 


Michael Cook

Editor

BioEdge

This week in BioEdge
by Michael Cook | May 14, 2016
Danish researchers post information on 70,000 people

by Xavier Symons | May 14, 2016
The International Society for Stem Cell Research (ISSCR) has released updated guidelines for stem cell research

by Xavier Symons | May 14, 2016
A woman in India thought to be in her 70s has given birth to a baby boy

by Xavier Symons | May 14, 2016
Limb surgery is becoming an increasingly common procedure among middle-class Indians.

by Michael Cook | May 14, 2016
Bioethicists claim that fear-based public health campaigns are effective and not necessarily stigmatising

by Michael Cook | May 14, 2016
But will it continue to supply them for euthanasia?

by Michael Cook | May 14, 2016
All the highlights

by Michael Cook | May 14, 2016
As Don De Lillo approaches 80, he meditates upon death
BioEdge

Suite 12A, Level 2 | 5 George St | North Strathfield NSW 2137 | Australia
Phone: +61 2 8005 8605
Mobile: 0422-691-615
New Media Foundation | Level 2, 5 George St | North Strathfield NSW 2137 | AUSTRALIA | +61 2 8005 8605 

No hay comentarios:

Publicar un comentario